
孤独症谱系障碍患儿静止脸试验发声行为特征分析
翟梦瑶, 仇娜娜, 唐传高, 翁娇, 章丽丽, 封敏, 肖湘, 肖婷, 郑文明, 柯晓燕
中国当代儿科杂志 ›› 2020, Vol. 22 ›› Issue (4) : 361-367.
孤独症谱系障碍患儿静止脸试验发声行为特征分析
Characteristics of vocalization in children with autism spectrum disorder during the still-face paradigm
目的 研究孤独症谱系障碍(ASD)患儿2岁前静止脸试验(SFP)发声行为特征及其与确诊时ASD症状严重程度之间的相关性。方法 前瞻性纳入43例7~23月龄可疑ASD患儿(可疑ASD组)及37例正常对照儿童(TD组),测量两组静止脸试验(SFP)下发声行为的时长及频率,随访至2岁,确诊34例ASD(ASD组)。使用孤独症诊断观察量表(ADOS)评估ASD组患儿症状严重程度,分析ASD组患儿2岁前发声行为特征及其与ASD症状严重程度的关系。结果 与TD组相比,ASD组SFP下有意义语言及朝向人发声时长及频率降低,朝向物发声时长增加(均P < 0.05)。Spearman相关分析显示,ASD组总发声、非语言发声、呀呀学语、朝向人发声、朝向物发声时长及频率与ADOS语言和沟通维度呈负相关(均P < 0.05);总发声、呀呀学语、朝向人发声时长及频率,朝向物发声时长与ADOS相互性社会互动维度呈负相关(均P < 0.05);总发声频率及呀呀学语时长、朝向人发声时长及频率与ADOS游戏维度呈负相关(均P < 0.05);总发声及非语言发声频率、朝向人发声时长及频率与ADOS刻板行为和局限兴趣维度呈负相关(均P < 0.05)。多元线性回归分析发现,总发声频率是ADOS语言和沟通维度的负向预测因子(P < 0.001);朝向人发声时长是ADOS相互性社会互动维度的负向预测因子(P < 0.05)。结论 SFP可以更好地突出ASD患儿2岁前的发声行为异常,且这些异常能够早期预测ASD症状的严重程度。
Objective To study the characteristics of vocalization during the still-face paradigm (SFP) before the age of 2 years and their correlation with the severity of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) symptoms at diagnosis in children with ASD. Methods A total of 43 children aged 7-23 months, who were suspected of ASD, were enrolled as the suspected ASD group, and 37 typical development (TD) children, aged 7-23 months, were enrolled as the TD group. The frequency and durations of vocalization in the SFP were measured. The children in the suspected ASD group were followed up to the age of 2 years, and 34 children were diagnosed with ASD. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) was used to assess the severity of symptoms. The correlation of the characteristics of vocalization before the age of 2 years with the severity of ASD symptoms was analyzed. Results Compared with the TD group, the ASD group had significant reductions in the frequency and durations of meaningful vocalization and vocalization towards people and a significant increase in the duration of vocalization toward objects (P < 0.05). The Spearman correlation analysis showed that in the ASD group, the frequency and durations of total vocalization, non-speech vocalization, babbling, vocalization towards people, and vocalization towards objects were negatively correlated with the score of communication in ADOS (P < 0.05). The frequency and durations of total vocalization, babbling, and vocalization towards people and the duration of vocalization towards objects were negatively correlated with the score of reciprocal social interaction in ADOS (P < 0.05). The frequency of total vocalization, the duration of babbling, and the frequency and duration of vocalization towards people were negatively correlated with the score of play in ADOS (P < 0.05). The frequency of total vocalization and non-speech vocalization and the frequency and durations of vocalization towards people were negatively correlated with the score of stereotyped behaviors and restricted interests in ADOS (P < 0.05). The multiple linear regression analysis showed that the frequency of total vocalization was a negative predictive factor for the score of communication in ADOS (P < 0.001), and the duration of vocalization towards people was a negative predictive factor for the score of reciprocal social interaction in ADOS (P < 0.05). Conclusions SFP can better highlight the abnormal vocalization of ASD children before the age of 2 years, and such abnormalities can predict the severity of ASD symptoms early.
Autism spectrum disorder / Still-face paradigm / Vocalization / Child
[1] 美国精神医学学会. 孤独症谱系障碍[M]//精神障碍诊断与统计手册(第5版). 张道龙, 刘春宇, 童慧琦, 等译. 北京:北京大学出版社, 2015:46-55.
[2] Herlihy L, Knoch K, Vibert B, et al. Parents' first concerns about toddlers with autism spectrum disorder:effect of sibling status[J]. Autism, 2015, 19(1):20-28.
[3] Handal AJ, Garcia Saavedra L, Schrader R, et al. Assessment of maternal-infant interaction:application of the Still Face Paradigm in a rural population of working women in Ecuador[J]. Matern Child Health J, 2017, 21(3):458-466.
[4] Yaari M, Rotzak NL, Mankuta D, et al. Preterm-infant emotion regulation during the still-face interaction[J]. Infant Behav Dev, 2018, 52:56-65.
[5] Müller M, Zietlow AL, Tronick E, et al. What dyadic reparation is meant to do:an association with infant cortisol reactivity[J]. Psychopathology, 2015, 48(6):386-399.
[6] Lambert-Brown BL, McDonald NM, Mattson WI, et al. Positive emotional engagement and autism risk[J]. Dev Psychol, 2015, 51(6):848-855.
[7] Ostfeld-Etzion S, Golan O, Hirschler-Guttenberg Y, et al. Neuroendocrine and behavioral response to social rupture and repair in preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders interacting with mother and father[J]. Mol Autism, 2015, 6:11.
[8] Swanson MR, Shen MD, Wolff JJ, et al. Naturalistic language recordings reveal "hypervocal" infants at high familial risk for autism[J]. Child Dev, 2018, 89(2):e60-e73.
[9] Roche L, Zhang D, Bartl-Pokorny KD, et al. Early vocal development in autism spectrum disorder, rett syndrome, and fragile X syndrome:insights from studies using retrospective video analysis[J]. Adv Neurodev Disord, 2018, 2(1):49-61.
[10] Chericoni N, de Brito Wanderley D, Costanzo V, et al. Prelinguistic vocal trajectories at 6-18 months of age as early markers of autism[J]. Front Psychol, 2016, 7:1595.
[11] Robins DL, Fein D, Barton ML, et al. The Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers:an initial study investigating the early detection of autism and pervasive developmental disorders[J]. J Autism Dev Disord, 2001, 31(2):131-144.
[12] Schopler E, Reichler RJ, DeVellis RF, et al. Toward objective classification of childhood autism:Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS)[J]. J Autism Dev Disord, 1980, 10(1):91-103.
[13] 周秉睿, 徐琼, 鲁萍, 等. 中文版《孤独症诊断观察量表》 模块1信度和效度评价及临床应用研究[J]. 中国循证儿科杂志, 2013, 8(4):257-261.
[14] Tronick E, Als H, Adamson L, et al. The infant's response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction[J]. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry, 1978, 17(1):1-13.
[15] Ozonoff S, Iosif AM, Baguio F, et al. A prospective study of the emergence of early behavioral signs of autism[J]. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 2010, 49(3):256-266.e1-2.
[16] Paul R, Fuerst Y, Ramsay G, et al. Out of the mouths of babes:vocal production in infant siblings of children with ASD[J]. J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 2011, 52(5):588-598.
[17] Werner E, Dawson G. Validation of the phenomenon of autistic regression using home videotapes[J]. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 2005, 62(8):889-895.
[18] Guo Y, Garfin DR, Ly A, et al. Emotion coregulation in motherchild dyads:a dynamic systems analysis of children with and without autism spectrum disorder[J]. J Abnorm Child Psychol, 2017, 45(7):1369-1383.
[19] Iverson JM. Early motor and communicative development in infants with an older sibling with autism spectrum disorder[J]. J Speech Lang Hear Res, 2018, 61(11):2673-2684.
[20] Giusti L, Provenzi L, Montirosso R. The Face-to-Face StillFace (FFSF) paradigm in clinical settings:socio-emotional regulation assessment and parental support with infants with neurodevelopmental disabilities[J]. Front Psychol, 2018, 9:789.
[21] Reck C, Van Den Bergh B, Tietz A, et al. Maternal avoidance, anxiety cognitions and interactive behaviour predicts infant development at 12 months in the context of anxiety disorders in the postpartum period[J]. Infant Behav Dev, 2018, 50:116-131.
[22] Asselmann E, Venz J, Wittchen HU, et al. Maternal anxiety and depressive disorders prior to, during and after pregnancy and infant interaction behaviors during the Face-to-Face Still Face Paradigm at 4 months postpartum:a prospective-longitudinal study[J]. Early Hum Dev, 2018, 122:45-53.
国家重点研发计划“重大慢性非传染性疾病防控研究”重点专项《儿童脑发育障碍的早期识别和综合干预》项目(2016 YFC1306205);江苏省科教强卫工程医学重点人才项目(ZDRCA2016076)。