Abstract:OBJECTIVE: To study the prevalence of iron deficiency in children between 6 months and 7 years and to study the diagnostic value of soluble transferrin receptor (sTfR) for iron deficiency in the children. METHODS: A total of 502 healthy children between 6 months and 7 years from Hangzhou City of Zhejiang Province were enrolled. Serum sTfR, serum ferritin (SF), serum iron (SI), total iron blinding capacity (TIBC), zinc protoporphyrin (ZPP), Hb, MCV and CRP levels were measured. RESULTS: The prevalence rate of iron deficiency was 19.5% in children at ages of 6 months to 7 years. The prevalence rate of iron deficiency was the highest in infants (≤1 year old; 34.7%), followed by in toddlers (1-3 years old; 19.4%) and preschoolers (3-7 years old; 14.0%). The mean serum sTfR level in infants (2.02±0.73 mg/L) was significantly higher than that in toddlers (1.68±0.40 mg/L) and preschoolers (1.67±0.29 mg/L) (P<0.05).The best cut-off value of serum sTfR for the diagnosis of iron deficiency was 2.02 mg/L in infants (sensitivity: 70.3%, specificity: 82.2%). The best cut-off value was 1.85 mg/L in toddlers (sensitivity: 71.7%; specificity: 86.4%), and that was 1.85 mg/L in preschoolers (sensitivity: 77.8%; specificity: 88.6%). Serum sTfR was correlated with SF (r=0.107, P<0.05), TIBC (r=0.276, P<0.01), TS (r=-0.139, P<0.05), ZPP (r=0.175, P<0.01) and MCV (r=-0.140, P<0.01). CONCLUSIONS: Iron deficiency is more prevalent in infants ≤1 year old. The mean serum level and the cut-off value of sTfR in infants are higher than in toddlers and preschoolers. Serum sTfR is an effective index for the diagnosis of iron deficiency in children, especially in infants≤ 1 year old.
WANG Ya-Ping,SHAO Jie,ZHUANG Xue-Ling. Value of soluble transferrin receptor in the diagnosis of iron deficiency in children[J]. CJCP, 2011, 13(7): 535-538.
[1]Lozoff B. Perinatal iron deficiency and the developing brain[J]. Pediatr Res, 2000, 48(2): 137-139.
[2]Lukowski AF, Koss M, Burden MJ, Jonides J, Nelson CA, Kaciroti N, et al. Iron deficiency in infancy and neurocognitive functioning at 19 years: evidence of long-term deficits in executive function and recognition memory[J]. Nutr Neurosci, 2010, 13(2): 54-70.
[3]Felt BT, Beard JL, Schallert T, Shao J, Aldridge JW, Connor JR, et al. Persistent neurochemical and behavioral abnormalities in adulthood despite early iron supplementation for perinatal iron deficiency anemia in rats[J]. Behav Brain Res, 2006, 171(2): 261-270.
[4]Beard JL, Erikson KM, Jones BC. Neurobehavioral analysis of developmental iron deficiency in rats[J]. Behav Brain Res, 2002, 134(1-2): 517-524.
[5]Golub MS, Hogrefe CE, Gennann SL, Capitanio JP, Lozoff B. Behavioral consequences of developmental iron deficiency in infant rhesus monkeys[J]. Neurotoxicol Teratol, 2006, 28(1): 3-17.
[7]White KC. Anemia is a poor predictor of iron deficiency among toddlers in the United States: for heme the bell tolls[J]. Pediatrics, 2005, 115(2): 315-320.
[10]Hay G, Refsum H, Whitelaw A, Melbye EL, Haug E, Borch-lohnsen B. Predictors of serum ferritin and serum soluble transferrin receptor in newborns and their associations with iron status during the first 2 y of life[J]. Am J Clin Nutr, 2007, 86(1): 64-73.
[11]Janus J, Moerschel SK. Evaluation of anemia in children[J].Am Fam Physician, 2010, 81(12): 1462-1471.